Saturday, March 3, 2007

Warning or Alarm

 NASA GISS Surface Temperature Analysis
The debate over Global Warming is a perfect example of how the integrity of scientific evidence and the credibility of science itself has been compromised in a highly divisive and partisan debate over policy.  The most visible proponents of each side present themselves as experts, present their own facts and ignore or undermine the facts of their adversaries.  Common ground based on accepted scientific evidence and practices disappears along with deliberative, public dialog.  Positions harden.  Real uncertainty and risk remain but disappear from view.  Subtleties are lost.  The public struggles to follow the debate and is mystified, confused.

Advocates on each side focus on short term benefits and political results.  Longer term education -- and a deeper public understanding of the issues -- is compromised.  When the public hears experts disagree they loose confidence in both sides.  Science and technology as a whole are diminished.

Debate of the the Nuclear Industry in the 1970's and 80's was another perfect case with similar consequences for science and technology.  The history of CFC damage to the ozone layer is another more recent example.  It is particularly tragic because now that we know that the alarm was real, that the intervention worked and that it was commercially very successful, the issue has passed from public view and science has missed the opportunity to regain lost ground.

Along the same lines many lessons have been learned from the disastrous limits to growth debate.  Yet in part because of the evidence collected in the process there is tremendous support for environmental protection, conservation and smaller family sizes throughout the industrialized world.  But the role played by scientists (not activists) in this global social movements and their contribution to what is now called sustainable development is still not universally acknowledged.

Lots of work has been done under the heading of Science, Technology and Society (STS) and the Public Understanding of Science as well.  One example of a comprehensive study is The House of Lords Select Committee on Science and Technology. Their |third report contains tremendous analysis of the tension between science, technology and society as well as what we can do about it.  (John Durant of the MIT Museum was somehow involved although I am unclear on his official role).

Warning or Alarm

The debate over global warming is a perfect example of how the integrity of scientific evidence and the credibility of science itself has been compromised in a highly divisive and partisan debate over policy.  The most visible proponents of each side present themselves as experts, present their own facts and ignore or undermine the facts of their adversaries.  Common ground based on accepted scientific evidence and practices disappears along with deliberative, public dialog.  Positions harden.  Real uncertainty and risk remain but disappear from view.  Subtleties are lost.  The public struggles to follow the debate and is mystified, confused.

Some interesting websites on Science and Society

Report from the House of Lords Committee on Science and Technology is an excellent assessment of the situation, analysis of root causes and recommendations for the future.  Published in 2000.

Strategies for Survival is aninteresting site by Bonnie Bucqueroux, who blogs about current threats and how we can respond at the personal and policy levels.  Great use of video and YouTube.There are dozens of Yahoo! Groups organized around energy issues.

Is it better to start a new group or join some other ones?  Or perhaps both?

Friday, March 2, 2007

The Challenge of Science and Civic Engagement

Many of the most complex and serious issues facing society today have significant technical and scientific dimensions: how to manage climate change, public health, loss of biodiversity, and dwindling natural resources, to name a few. These are not just technical or scientific issues, however.  These are fundamentally social, political and economic problems with substantial ethical, moral and cultural dimensions.  Answers to these challenges require judgment, values and priorities in addition to empirical observation, experimental evidence, quantitative models and methods.

Wednesday, February 14, 2007

Brain Physiology of Love and Sex

Elizabeth Cohen posted this story on CNN about what cognitive scientists are learning about love.

http://www.cnn.com/2007/HEALTH/02/14/love.science/

We exchanged a few emails on the subject so I figured I'd take one of them and post this blog entry.

Cohen wrote, “In a group of experiments, Dr. Lucy Brown, a professor in the department of neurology and neuroscience at the Albert Einstein College of Medicine in New York, and her colleagues did MRI brain scans on college students who were in the throes of new love.  While being scanned, the students looked at a photo of their beloved. The scientists found that the caudate area of the brain -- which is involved in cravings -- became very active. Another area that lit up: the ventral tegmental, which produces dopamine, a powerful neurotransmitter that affects pleasure and motivation."

"Dr. Brown said scientists believe that when you fall in love, the ventral tegmental floods the caudate with dopamine. The caudate then sends signals for more dopamine....  The more dopamine you get, the more of a high you feel" similar to the effect of cocaine on the nervous system.

The physiology of sex, on the other hand, appears to be quite different.  Cohen explained, “In studies when researchers showed erotic photos to people as they underwent brain scans, they found activity in the hypothalamus and amygdala areas of the brain. The hypothalamus controls drives like hunger and thirst and the amygdala handles arousal, among other things.  In the studies of people in love, 'we didn't find activity in either,' according to Dr. Fisher, an anthropologist and author of Why We Love -- the Nature and Chemistry of Romantic Love.

Let’s examine their findings in more detail and ask some questions.  First of all, how do we know the students they selected were actually in love?  And how do we know that looking at pictures of your beloved evokes the same response as love?  And finally, can we trust their conclusions?

This kind of questioning is a huge part of science.  Is the methodology valid?  Scientists are skeptical.  Unfortunately journalists are less so.  They rarely discuss questions on the methodology of the study or delve into the philosophical underpinnings of the conclusions and report on how scientists challenge one another.  Would that be boring?  Maybe.  But without that discussion, how do we know, really?  And I wonder if you could make it interesting....

In their search for a juicy story, science journalism is often guilty of exaggerating the claims or conclusions of the scientists themselves.  For example, in this case they might have concluded that brain region A was activated when the subject studied images of X while region B was activated when images Y were projected to the subject.  This deductive reasoning is solid:  if A then X and not Y.  If B then Y and not X.   It’s what makes reductionist science work.

But that’s not where it ends.  A good paper might cautiously extend limited conclusions like these beyond the scope of the experiment back to the considerably more complex “real world.”   The synthesis of these results with lots of other experiments on mind, brain AND body systems is actually inductive reasoning.  These results together with lots of other results might support (and do not contradict) a proposed model that accounts for the mechanisms of perception and other kinds of cognitive behavior.  We can’t say that the model is right.  We can only say that it has not been disproved yet.

Naturally, we would have to find and study the original paper to be sure of this.  But it does serve to illustrate this point:  reporters typically skip this intermediate step making it seem like the experiment is deductive reasoning about the real world.  It is not.

Implicit messages and omissions are another problem with scientific journalism.  Cohen wrote, “By studying MRI brain scans of people newly in love, scientists are learning a lot about the science of love: Why love is so powerful, and why being rejected is so horribly painful.”  To the romantic reader, lover of poetry, art or music perhaps, claims like this imply much more than is actually intended by the scientists themselves.  Knowledge of physiology tells us nothing about the subjective  experience of love at all and certainly can’t help us answer questions like “Why should we experience such feelings?”

Reasonable, well adjusted scientists are well aware that the model of the mechanism is interesting but totally distinct from the experience or the meaning of the phenomenon.  E. O. Wilson would suggest that’s exactly why science and literature and art and music are complimentary or even 'consilient' inductions insofar as they 'jump together' and reinforce one another.  It is assumed by scientists. Perhaps we should make it explicit for the rest of us as we ask, "but how do we know?"

Monday, December 11, 2006

Small Groups Needed to Solve Problems in Science and Society

Margaret Mead once said, "Never doubt that a small group of thoughtful, committed citizens can change the world. Indeed, it's the only thing that ever has."

But where are these small groups of thoughtful, committed citizens confronting the really large problems facing science and society in the 21st Century: climate change, loss of biodiversity, diminished natural resources and the challenges of sustainable development in a shrinking, hungry planet, to name a few?

Although such small groups are constantly forming around us, and many of them are fully engaged on parts of the puzzle, somehow, with respect to these systemic problems as a whole, they fail to coalesce, gain traction and drive change.  Why?

Friday, December 1, 2006

Public Attitudes about Science in the UK

The following list was published in the third report of the House of Lord's Select Committee on Science and Society. It was published in 2000 but it still rings true today.
  • The perceived purpose of science is crucial to the public response.
  • People now question all authority, including scientific authority.
  • People place more trust in science which is seen as "independent".
  • There is still a culture of governmental and institutional secrecy in the United Kingdom, which invites suspicion.
  • Some issues currently treated by decision-makers as scientific issues in fact involve many other factors besides science. Framing the problem wrongly by excluding moral, social, ethical and other concerns invites hostility.
  • What the public finds acceptable often fails to correspond with the objective risks as understood by science. This may relate to the degree to which individuals feel in control and able to make their own choices.
  • Underlying people's attitudes to science are a variety of values. Bringing these into the debate and reconciling them are challenges for the policy-maker.

COMMENTS from the original blog

2006-12-01 12:47:40 stefano
Would Americans Hold the Same Attitudes
I wonder, for example, if Americans have the same degree of trust in science?  Is American science seen as independent as that of the UK?